Losing, Not Violent Content, Triggers Video Game Rage

Hostile behavior on the part of video gamers may be due to feelings of failure and frustration during play, rather than a game’s violent content.
Losing, Not Violent Content, Triggers Video Game Rage
"When people feel they have no control over the outcome of a game, that leads to aggression," says Richard Ryan. "We saw that in our experiments. If you press someone's competencies, they'll become more aggressive, and our effects held up whether the games were violent or not." Shutterstock*
Updated:

Hostile behavior on the part of video gamers may be due to feelings of failure and frustration during play, rather than a game’s violent content.

A new study is the first to look at the player’s psychological experience with video games instead of focusing solely on the game’s content. Failure to master a game and its controls can lead to frustration and aggression, regardless of whether the game is violent or not, researchers say.

“Any player who has thrown down a remote control after losing an electronic game can relate to the intense feelings or anger failure can cause,” says lead author Andrew Przybylski, a researcher at the Oxford Internet Institute at Oxford University. That frustration is commonly known among gamers as “rage-quitting.”

The experience is not unique to gaming, says coauthor Richard Ryan, a motivational psychologist at the University of Rochester. For example, in sports, players may lose a game as a result of a bad call.

“When people feel they have no control over the outcome of a game, that leads to aggression,” he says. “We saw that in our experiments. If you press someone’s competencies, they’ll become more aggressive, and our effects held up whether the games were violent or not.”

To tease out which aspects of the gaming experience lead to aggressive feelings, the researchers manipulated the interface, controls, and degree of difficulty in custom-designed video games across six lab experiments. Nearly 600 college-aged participants were tasked with playing the games—many of which included violent and nonviolent variations—and then were tested for aggressive thoughts, feelings, or behaviors.

Threats to Our Ego

In one experiment, undergraduates held their hand in a bowl of painfully cold water for 25 seconds. They were led to believe that the length of time was determined by a prior participant, but in fact, all participants were assigned the same duration.

Next, participants were randomly asked to play either a simple or challenging version of Tetris, after which they were asked to assign the amount of time a future participant would have to leave their hand in the chilled water. Players who experienced the difficult Tetris game assigned on average 10 seconds more of chilled water pain to subsequent players than those who played the easy version.

In all the experiments, it wasn’t the narrative or imagery, but the lack of mastery of the game’s controls and the degree of difficulty players had completing the game that led to frustration. The study demonstrates that aggression is a negative side effect of the frustration felt while playing the video game, Ryan says. “When the experience involves threats to our ego, it can cause us to be hostile and mean to others.”

The researchers also surveyed 300 avid gamers to identify how real world gamers might experience the same phenomena. When asked about pre- and post-game feelings, gamers reported that their inability to master a game or its controls caused feelings of frustration and affected their sense of enjoyment in the experience.

Published online in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, the findings offer an important contribution to the debate about the effects of violent video games. Many critics of video games have been premature in their conclusions that violent video games cause aggression, Ryan says.

“It’s a complicated area, and people have simplistic views.” Nonviolent games like Tetris or Candy Crush can leave players as, if not more, aggressive than games with violence, if they’re poorly designed or too difficult.

Source: University of Rochester. Republished from Futurity.org under Creative Commons License 3.0.

*Image of “man playing video game“ via Shutterstock

Melissa Greco Lopes
Melissa Greco Lopes
Author
Related Topics